Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on self-help groups and peer pressure as savings commitment device

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on self-help groups and peer pressure as savings commitment device"

Transcription

1 Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on self-help groups and peer pressure as savings commitment device Felipe Kast Stephan Meier Dina Pomeranz PRELIMINARY VERSION. COMMENTS VERY WELCOME! Abstract Peer groups are widely observed to be used as commitment devices to reach personal goals, but there is little empirical evidence evaluating their effectiveness and analyzing what aspect leads to their success. We conduct two randomized field experiments among low-income micro-entrepreneurs in Chile to shed light on these questions. We find that self-help peer groups increase the number of deposits in a formal savings account 3.5-fold, and almost double the average savings balance. Comparing this effect to a separate treatment with an increased interest rate, we estimate that a commensurate effect would require increasing the interest rate by about 7.8 percentage points, and find that most participants do not respond to the increased interest rate at all. A second field experiment shows that over 80% of this effect can be achieved without actual meetings or peer pressure, through simple text messages that remind participants of their savings commitment, and give them feedback on their achievement. A text message coupled with peer pressure by a real-life savings buddy has no larger effect than a text message that simply informs participants of their own achievement and the success rate of others. These findings suggest that regular follow-up and taking stock may be more important than actual peer pressure in making peer group meetings an effective tool to encourage savings. We thank Alberto Alesina, Shawn Cole, David Cutler, Michael Kremer, Lakshmi Iyer, David Laibson, Brigitte Madrian, Sendhil Mullainathan and Alvin Roth for helpful comments and discussions. We are grateful to Fondo Esperanza, Banco Credichile and Microdatos for outstanding collaboration in the implementation process. This project would not have been possible without the generous support by the following institutions: the Ford Foundation, the Lab for Economic Applications and Policy (LEAP) at Harvard, the Woman and Public Policy Program at the Harvard Kennedy School, the David Rockefeller Center for Latin American Studies, the Chazen Institute of International Business at Columbia Business School, the Columbia University CIBER, and the Russell Sage Foundation Small Grants Program. Pontificia Universidad Catolica de Chile, Avda. Vicuna Mackenna 4860, Macul, Santiago, Chile Columbia University, Graduate School of Business, 710 Uris Hall, 3022 Broadway, New York, NY 10027; sm3087@columbia.edu. MIT, Poverty Action Lab, 30 Wadsworth Street, Cambridge, MA 02142; pomeranz@mit.edu. 1

2 1 Introduction Many people have difficulty following through with plans that involve immediate costs and delayed benefits such as quitting smoking, losing weight, going to the gym, reducing debt levels or sticking to savings goals. As a consequence, a large segment of the population is unsatisfied with their behavior in certain areas. For savings outcomes, for example, a substantial proportion of the population in developed and developing countries report that they are unhappy with their current savings amount and regret not having saved more (Choi et al., 2002, for the US, authors survey results for Chile). A growing body of literature in economics focuses on individuals self-control problem or the degree of their present-biased time preferences as part of the problem (e.g. Angeletos et al., 2001; Laibson, 1997; O Donoghue and Rabin, 1999). Much of this past work analyses the decisions of a single decision maker. While the decisions involved are often individualistic in principal, they are critically influenced by peers. In fact, the use of peers as a commitment device to overcome self-control problems is a widely observed phenomenon. Self-help peer groups, such as Alcoholics Anonymous or weight-loss groups, enjoy widespread popularity and claim to help people overcome their self-control problems. 1 There is, however, no clean evidence investigating whether self-help peer groups actually help overcome self-control problems and what aspect of peer groups is most critical for their effectiveness. 2 This paper analyzes the effect of self-help peer groups for a financial goal: increasing savings. We conduct two randomized field experiments among low-income micro-entrepreneurs in Chile to test the power of self-help peer groups in helping peo- 1 For example, AA has 2.1m members world-wide, 1.3 of them in the US ( and each week an average of 1.3m participants attend a Weight Watchers meeting ( 2 The only study on self-help peer groups we are aware of is a medical study by Walsh et al. (1991) which compares AA meetings with hospital treatment. The effect of AA meetings per se was not tested, however. 2

3 ple increase their savings. The effect of self-help peer groups is particularly important for saving behavior for at least two reasons. First, overcoming self-control problems is key in helping people reach their own goals. Many people report regretting not having saved more. At the same time, a series of mechanisms that help overcoming self-control problems have been shown to be effective in increasing savings (e.g. Madrian and Shea, 2001; Thaler and Benartzi, 2004). Second, most of these mechanisms, such as defaults and direct deposits from wages, have been found to be effective for people working in the formal labor market in developed countries. However, most of these mechanisms cannot be applied to those working in the informal sector or to independent entrepreneurs. Alternative mechanisms are therefore important, especially in developing countries where a large share of the population does not have a formal wage bill. Two randomized field experiments shed light on the power of self-help peer groups as a commitment device and investigate the importance of actual peers for their effectiveness. Our first experiment tests the effectiveness of self-help peer groups to help individuals increase their savings. It is conducted among 2,700 members of Fondo Esperanza, a Chilean microfinance organization. Their members are microentrepreneurs and meet on a weekly basis in groups. 68% of participants say they frequently regret not having saved more. We randomly select some groups to be offered savings accounts with a self-help peer group component. Individuals in these groups have an opportunity to publicly declare their weekly deposit goal in their group. Fulfilling their weekly goals is subsequently monitored in the regular meetings, and compliance is rewarded with public acknowledgement in the group. No monetary incentives for compliance are provided. We compare the savings outcomes of individuals in the self-help group treatment with a control group that was only offered the savings account without a self-help group component, and with a third set of groups that was offered savings accounts 3

4 with a substantially increased interest rate. The treatment with the increased interest rate serves as a benchmark to compare the effect of the self-help peer groups with a traditional approach to increase savings. To our knowledge, this is the first study evaluating the impact of a peer commitment device on the use of savings accounts, and the first experimental study to evaluate the impact of interest rates on savings. 3 The results show that self-help peer groups help increase savings substantially. The number of deposits per month increases more than 3.5-fold and the amounts saved almost double. We compare the effect of peer groups to the more traditional means of fostering savings, an interest rate increase. Linearly extrapolating from a treatment in which we increase the interest rate from 0.3% to 5% indicates that the effect of selfhelp peer groups on average amounts saved corresponds to increase in the interest rate by about 7.8 percentage points. However, this comparison underestimates the relative effectiveness of the peer group treatment for most people: Looking at the quantile effects reveals that self-help peer groups affect a much larger share of participants than an interest rate increase. While only a small number of savers take advantage of the increased interest rate, the self-help peer groups help a much larger share of participants increase their savings. Conceptually, the impact of self-help peer groups as a commitment device for savings can be due to various features of those groups. The regular meetings of peers can act as a commitment device, where the other group members function as an enforcement mechanism of a contract or a binding implicit agreement to stick to a 3 Beshears et al. (2009) study the effect of informing employees of a US firm of other employees 401(k) plan participation. Their results show that peer information increases savings only for certain employees and not for others. Ashraf et al. (2006b) analyze a commitment mechanism in the Philippines that allow for withdrawals of savings only after a certain date, after a certain amount has been accumulated, or for a specific purpose. They show that such a commitment device can increase savings and is particularly attractive for present-biased individuals. The same authors also conduct a study that analyses the effect of deposit collectors as a commitment device in The Phillipines (Ashraf et al., 2006a). They investigate the determinants of participation for a deposit collection service for micro-savers in the Philippines and find that distance to the branch of the bank, a measure of transaction costs, and marriage status, a measure of intra-household dynamics, have significant predictive power on participation. 4

5 savings plan. Such peer pressure can help achieve one s savings goal either through direct peer punishment of deviation from a resolution or indirectly, if deviations from following one s plan negatively impacting a person s reputation and image (Bernheim (for a similar argument about norm adherence, see, e.g. 1994) and Ariely et al. (for image motivation, see 2009); Benabou and Tirole (for image motivation, see 2006). Mas and Moretti (2009) show in a different setting, namely a work environment, that peer monitoring can have substantial behavioral effects. The effect is particularly strong if the peers know each other well, i.e. they have experienced past interactions and expect future interactions. If peer pressure is the main factor driving the impact of self-help peer groups, then physical meetings may be key to the success of peer groups. At a minimum, the behavior of an individual has to be observed by at least one other peer. However, there might be mechanisms other than peer pressure that make peer groups effective. Self-help peer groups might affect savings because they provide information (Battaglini et al., 2005), trigger a competitive trait to win (e.g. Fliessbach et al. (2007) and Cooper and Fan (2008)) or function as a reminder (Karlan et al., 2010). Battaglini et al. (2005) show theoretically that observing others who stick to their plan can be motivating. However, the only paper we are aware of on the effects of conveying information about peers behavior on savings by Beshears et al. (2009) shows that this channel is delicate and can even backfire (see also Schultz et al., 2007). Karlan et al. (2010) show that monthly text message reminders can increase savings. If self-help peer groups operate mainly through learning about the behavior of others, or as reminders, then physical meetings might not be as important required, and in the latter case, actual peers are not required at all. While many factors may be important for the impact of self-help peer groups, unbundling the effect helps not only to better understand the effect of peers in decisions with self-control problems, it also allows to investigate which aspects of the 5

6 successful self-help peer groups are crucial and which are dispensable. This has important policy implications. While effective, regular peer meetings are also relatively cumbersome and may only appeal to a narrow population. Their scalability might therefore be limited. Unbundling different elements of peers as a commitment device allows distilling their most powerful elements, and can give guidance for the choice of more cost-effective, scalable delivery models. Our second experiment is the first attempt to unbundle the effect of peers as a commitment device, by investigating the importance of actual peer meetings and peer pressure for their effectiveness. This field experiment is implemented in the same setting as the peer group experiment. It consists of two treatments and a control group that use text messages instead of actual group meetings. Similar to the peer group meetings, the text messages provide regular follow-up to the participant, giving them weekly feedback on whether or not they fulfilled on their commitment to make a weekly deposit. The first treatment combines the regular follow-up through the text message with peer pressure by a real-life Savings Buddy : Participants choose a person that will encourage them to stick to their savings plan. Both the savings buddy and the participant then receive a weekly text message, informing them whether or not the participant made a deposit this week. The message sent to the participant also reminds the participant that the savings buddy received the same information. This peer pressure treatment is compared to the control group, which receives no text messages, and to a second text-message treatment with no savings buddy. Like in the peer pressure treatment, in the second text-message treatment the participants receive a weekly text message, informing them whether or not they made their promised weekly deposit. Instead of a reminder that their savings buddy received information about their performance, this group receives information about how many other participants made their weekly deposit. 6

7 Comparing the two treatments together to the control group that received no text messages sheds light on the question whether actual in-person meetings are an indispensable element for the finding that self-help peer groups are a powerful device to increase savings. The results show that regular follow-up through text messages can capture a significant part of what makes peer group meetings effective as a commitment device: Our calculations indicate that over 80% of the effect of meetings can be achieved by peer-related text messages. Receiving a weekly text message doubles the number of monthly deposits and increases the average amount of new savings from zero to almost 8000 Pesos. These findings indicate that regular in-person meetings are not an indispensable feature of the power of self-help peer groups and that modern technologies can be employed to play the role of synthetic self-help groups, providing strong effects that may be much more scalable than in-person meetings. Comparing the effect of the Savings Buddy -treatment to the treatment of text messages without peer pressure gives insights into the role of peer pressure for the effectiveness of self-help peer groups as a commitment device. The results show that peer pressure through a savings buddy has no larger effect than receiving a text message with feedback about one s own achievement and information about the success rate of others. This suggests that actual peer pressure, through fear of embarrassment or desire for positive signaling, is not the main aspect of what makes self-help peer groups effective. Regular reminders coupled with information on how one compares to others turn out to be just as powerful. Combined with the results of Beshears et al. (2009), who find information about peers savings performance not to be very effective in increasing savings, and the results of Karlan et al. (2010), who find that reminders positively affect savings, our results suggest that while self-help peer groups are very effective in increasing savings, their effectiveness may be less due to the social interaction and peer pressure than 7

8 commonly assumed, and more due to the regular follow-up and feedback mechanism that they provide. The remainder of the paper is organized as follows: Section 2 presents the set-up and the design of both field experiments. Section 3 presents the result of Experiment 1 on the effect of self-help peer groups on savings. Section 4 presents evidence from Experiment 2 using text messages to test the importance of peer meetings and peer pressure on savings. Section 5 concludes. 2 Background, Data, and Design of Experiments 2.1 Background and Data Both randomized field experiments for this study are conducted in collaboration with two partner institutions: The microfinance institution Fondo Esperanza (FE), and a large private bank, Banco Credichile. The context of FE is particularly suitable to study the role that peer groups can play as a commitment device for those who do not have access to the types of commitment devices that have been found to be effective for employees in the formal sector. Members of FE are self-employed micro-entrepreneurs (e.g. street vendors, cosmetic saleswomen), many of whom work in the informal sector. About 90% of FE members are women, and most live and work in urban areas. Members of FE meet regularly, on a weekly or biweekly basis, in groups of peers, together with a group monitor from FE. This feature allows us to incorporate the peer group based commitment structure, in order to study the impact of selfhelp peer groups on savings. The purpose of the meetings is to enforce the regular repayment of the micro-loans that participants receive from FE in 3-month cycles. The loans are intended for investment in their micro-enterprise, and the credit cycles 8

9 as well as the immediate and regular repayments are on a rigid schedule that does not provide liquidity for irregular or unexpected financial needs. A large portion of participants therefore express the desire to build savings that are available as a buffer stock for emergencies or towards non-business related goals. This is the main reason expressed for why participants are interested in saving and borrowing at the same time. The savings accounts that are offered to clients of FE as part of the randomized experiments are with the commercial bank, Banco Credichile, because FE is not a licensed bank and is therefore not legally allowed to hold savings deposits. The accounts offered to participants have no maintenance fee or minimum balance. Most accounts yield a 0.3% real interest rate, with a special 5% real interest rate granted to a randomly selected sub-sample of the participants (discussed below). We draw on three different sources of data: data on our key outcomes of interest, namely take-up and use of the savings accounts, is directly obtained from Banco Credichile, the bank holding the accounts. It includes information on which participants open an account, whether they use the account after opening it, and the dates and amounts of all withdrawals and deposits made after opening the account. The second source of data comes from administrative files that FE collected about its members. This data includes information about estimated household size and income, participants years of education, as well as the amount of credit with FE. Finally, we complement these two sources of administrative data with an extensive baseline and follow-up survey. The baseline survey was conducted prior to the introduction of the savings accounts in April-May 2008 during one of the group meetings. The follow-up survey was conducted one year after the introduction of the savings accounts, in June-July 2008, through individual interviews. All surveys were administered by Centro de Microdatos from the University of Chile. In the context of the follow-up survey, we randomized participants into the treatments of the text message 9

10 experiment (see below). 2.2 Experiment 1: Self-Help Peer Groups Design Our first experiment analyzes the effect of self-help peer groups on savings and is conducted among 196 groups with a total of 2,682 members of the microfinance organization Fondo Esperanza (FE). Before the start of the intervention, a baseline survey is first conducted in April/May It is administered to the groups in one of their regular meetings and filled out by each participant individually. In the weeks following the baseline survey, one of three types of savings accounts with the commercial bank Banco Credichile is introduced to the groups, with an offer to open such an account. The universe of study participants consists of all members of the 196 groups who are in the meeting when the baseline survey is conducted. Groups are randomly assigned to one of the three types of savings accounts and all members of each group are offered only one of the three types of savings accounts, without knowing the existence of the other types of accounts. All three types of accounts are attractive compared to other options in the market in that they have no maintenance fees, no minimum balance, and the possibility is offered to go to the bank together with other interested members of the group in order to open the account together. Among those offered a savings account, half are randomly selected for the self-help group feature as a commitment device. The other half is only offered a savings account without any group support beyond the opening of the account. Among those who did not receive the peer group commitment structure, half receive a basic account, the other half receive an increased interest rate of 5%. The types of accounts have the following features: 10

11 1. Basic savings account: The basic savings account is an individual savings account, with a real annual interest rate of 0.3% (similar to the highest available alternative in the Chilean market), no maintenance fees, no minimum balance, and a two dollar minimum opening deposit. General conditions are the same across all accounts: savings are completely liquid for withdrawal at any time and the conditions are guaranteed for a minimum duration of two years. 2. Self-help group account: This account is identical to the basic account but with an added accountability structure, such that the weekly peer group meetings act as a self-help peer group in the following way: group members have the option of publicly announcing to the group what their weekly savings goal is for the coming credit cycle (approximately 3 months). Subsequently, members verify in each group meeting who complied with their own savings goal and who did not. Those who complied and show a deposit slip to prove it receive a sticker in a booklet. Those who collect enough stickers receive a diploma as a non-monetary award. 3. High interest account: The high interest rate account is identical to the basic account, but offers a 5% real interest rate instead of 0.3%. It is explicitly presented to the group as the savings account with the highest yield in the market, and the concept of interest rates and compounded interest is explained to participants in the meeting when the account is introduced. Summary Statistics Table 1 presents summary statistics for the 196 groups in the sample of the peer group experiment. As expected given the random assignment, average characteristics in the groups assigned to different treatments are very similar, and there are no statistically significant differences with the exception of group size. [Table 1 about here.] 11

12 Participants in the study have an average of 9.65 years of schooling and their mean age is 43 years. Monthly income per capita of their households is 84,212 pesos (about 160 USD), with an average household size of 4.3 people. 68% of participants did not have a savings account prior to the study. The mean total savings for those who did have a pre-existing account is 69,108 pesos. While the income is expressed in per capita terms, these savings may be the combined savings of several household members, especially including participants children. Participants mean debt, including the micro-loan from FE, was 408,312 pesos. The larger amounts of debt compared to savings is not surprising given that participants are entrepreneurs and most of their debt is backed up by inventories and future sales. The average number of people who were present the day of the baseline survey is 14.7 per group, with a slightly lower average in groups offered the basic savings account. For the questions about attitudes toward savings and toward their peers in FE, as well as participants discount rate and hyperbolic discounting, we conduct an F-test, which clearly rejects the null hypothesis that they are jointly significant in predicting whether a group has been assigned to a basic account or one of the other two accounts. 2.3 Experiment 2: Text Messages Follow-Up Design The second experiment of our study is conducted among 873 participants who had opened an account with Banco Credichile in the scope of the peer group experiment. Experiment 2 starts a little over a year later. During the individual follow-up survey to the peer group experiment, eligible participants are randomly offered one of two text message services aimed at helping them reach their personal savings goals, or are assigned to the control group: 12

13 1. Peer Pressure treatment: One third of eligible participants are offered a Savings- Buddy Text Message Service. It simulates the aspect of the regular follow-up of peer group meetings through a weekly text message, and combines it with peer pressure by a real-life Savings Buddy : The participants set a weekly savings goal for themselves. They then choose a person that will encourage them to stick to their savings plan as their savings buddy. Both the savings buddy and the participant subsequently receive a weekly text message, informing them whether or not the participant made a deposit this week. The message sent to the participant also reminds the participant that the savings buddy received the same information Savings Information treatment: In the same way as in the peer pressure treatment, participants of the Savings Information Text Message Service are asked to set a weekly savings goal for themselves, and receive a weekly text message, informing them whether or not they made their weekly deposit. However, there is no savings buddy exerting pressure on them to comply, and instead of a reminder that their savings buddy received information about their performance, this group is told how many other participants made their weekly deposit. 3. Control treatment: A control group is asked in the same way as the two treatment groups to set themselves a weekly savings goal, but is not offered either of the two text-message services. Sample selection and set-up of the intervention Prior to administering the survey, we randomly assigned all participants of Experiment 1 who had opened a savings account to one of the three text message treatment groups described above. The randomization was stratified by savings balance in the accounts with Banco Credichile prior to the intervention and by the peer groups 4 For exact wording of the messages, see Appendix. 13

14 within FE to which the participants belonged. This latter stratification automatically assures stratification by type of savings account of Experiment 1. In order to maximize the take-up rate, prior to being offered a text message service during the survey, study participants, including the control group, were asked a set of screening questions, to determine who would remain in this study. First, potential participants were asked whether they owned a cell phone. Only those who had a cell phone (85.2% of all participants who had opened a savings account) were told about a new cell phone service designed to help people reach their savings goals. This service would offer people the option to receive weekly text messages reminding them of their savings goal and motivating them to reach it. Participants were then asked whether in the hypothetical case that such a service was provided for free, would they be interested in participating. Only those who responded yes to this question (69.5% of participants with cell phones) were maintained in the study. This process led to our sample of 873 participants in the text message experiment. These 873 study participants, including the control group, were asked how much they would aim to save over the next three months if such a service were offered and how much that would translate into in terms of weekly targets. Those who were offered one of the two services were then informed that they had really been selected to participate for free in the service for three months, and the details of their particular service ( Savings Buddy or Savings Information ) were explained to them. If they chose to actually participate in the service, they were then asked for their phone number. Those in the Savings Buddy service were additionally told to choose a Savings Buddy, and were instructed to choose someone who would motivate them towards their savings goal. The name and phone number of that person were subsequently registered. Finally, participants were asked to state their relationship to their buddy and the reason they selected that individual. Participants who signed up for the service were subsequently called on the phone 14

15 to ensure that their phone number was working, that they understood the procedures, and that all their outstanding questions were answered. The research team matched weekly data from the bank with individuals in the study and sent corresponding text messages to participants. The text message service started as soon as the researchers obtained the data from the interviews, and the preparation procedures described above were completed. Since the interviews happened in a staggered manner over time, different participants started receiving the service at different points in time. However, the service ended for everyone at the same time at the end of October Summary Statistics Table 2 presents summary statistics for the 873 participants in our sample (i.e. those who said they would be interested in principle in the service if it were offered). As expected given the random assignment, average characteristics in the groups assigned to different treatments are very similar. [Table 2 about here.] Mean monthly per capita income of participants household is about 117,000 Chilean Pesos, or about 230 USD, with an average of 4.4 members in each household. Similar to the sample in the peer group experiment their mean age is 44 years, with an education of 9.6 years on average. Also very similar to the sample in the peer group experiment, seventy percent of participants donõt have a savings account prior to the account they opened in the context of this study. The average savings balance in those accounts at the beginning of the text message experiment is 14,853 pesos or about 30 USD. The average number of monthly deposits in 2008 is 0.18 and average number of withdrawals is Take-up rates of the two treatments is very similar. 42.2% of participants in the sample of those who expressed interest in the service end up actually signing up when it was indeed offered to them. This number is only very slightly different between the 15

16 two treatments, with 42.8% for the Savings Buddy service and 41.6% for the Savings Information treatment. In the following, we show the results of both experiments. Section 3 presents evidence on the impact of self-help peer groups for saving, compared to a regular savings account and to a high interest rate account. In section 4, we show the results of the text message intervention, which shed light on the questions of whether peer pressure and actual in-person meetings are the crucial element of what makes self-help peer groups effective as a commitment device work. 3 The Effect of Self-Help Peer Groups on Savings The results of Experiment 1 are presented in two steps: In a first step, we look at the overall Intent-to-Treat (ITT) and the Treatment-on-the-Treated (TOT) effect of self-help peer groups on deposits and amount saved. In a second step, we compare the impact of self-help peer groups to the effectiveness of a substantial increase in the interest rate in order to get a sense of the magnitude of the peer group effect compared to a more traditional means to increase savings. 3.1 Self-Help Peer Groups Figure 1 shows the ITT effect of self-help groups on deposit and savings behavior compared to a basic savings account. The figure shows savings outcomes for 12 months after the experiment started. It is clear from the figure that self-help peer groups increase savings outcomes, measured in two ways: Panel A of Figure 1 shows the effect of self-help peer groups on the number of deposits per month. The figure indicates that the average number of deposits is almost 4-times higher for people who were randomly assigned to a self-help group compared to people who were not. The effect is very strong in the first few months 16

17 after self-help peer groups were formed. But while it decreases over time, even in the end of the year, the number of deposits is still 3.5-times higher compared to the control group. [Figure 1] Panel B of Figure 1 shows that self-help peer groups not only increase the number of deposits but actually lead to higher savings balances. It displays the mean balance in the savings account for those in the peer treatment and the control group. Average balance is 2-times higher for people in self-help peer groups compared to the basic account. The effect persists over time and the difference does not decrease during a whole first year after the introduction of the self-help groups. We estimate the following regressions to further test whether the effect is significant. This will also allow us to extend the specification and compare the effect to the high interest accounts. S i = α + β 1 Self Help i + β 2 Interest Rate i + ɛ i (1) S i is the savings outcome for individual i. We will analyze three savings outcomes: (1) the average monthly number of deposits, (2) the average monthly deposited amount, and (3) the average balance (total deposits minus withdrawals). In order to illustrate the effect of outliers, we show the results for the mean of the deposited amount and the savings balance for the whole sample as well as winsorized at top 1% and top 5%. Self Help is a dummy variable equal to 1 for individuals in the self-help peer groups and 0 otherwise. Interest Rate is a dummy variable equal to 1 if the individual has a savings account with a high interest rate and 0 otherwise. ɛ is an error term. 5 5 The results do not change in any significant way if we also control for additional characteristics. 17

18 Table 3 shows the effect of self-help peer groups on savings outcomes. Panel A presents the ITT effect for all three outcomes. The regression results support the results in Figure 1: The number of deposits, the amount deposited and the savings balance is significantly higher for the self-help peer group treatment than in the control group that was only offered a basic account. Panel B in Table 3 shows the TOT effects of self-help peer groups. Take-up rates of the savings accounts are very similar between the treatments: 50% for the basic account, 51% for the High Interest account and 55% for the self-help peer groups (none of the differences is statistically significant). We would therefore expect the TOT effects to be approximately double the ITT effects. Panel B shows that indeed the effects are about twice the size. In sum, the evidence indicates that the self-help peer groups are effective in encouraging deposits by participants, which in turn leads to a substantial increased average balance in the accounts. The increased number of deposits is therefore not offset by a corresponding increase in withdrawals even though savings accounts are fully liquid, and withdrawals are not observed by the peers. 3.2 Self-Help Peer Groups vs. Interest Rate Increase To get a sense of the magnitude of the effect of self-help peer groups on savings, we compare it to the impact of a more classical treatment to encourage saving. In a separate treatment, the interest rate is increased to 5% annually in real terms. This is substantially higher than the 0.3% offered in the self-help peer treatment and in the basic account. Figure 2 compares the effect of self-help groups to the high interest account and the basic account. The four panels show the treatment effects on the mean savings balances as well as the 75, 95, and 99 percentile (the median is zero, given that the take-up is only about 50%). The panels show at least three interesting patterns: 18

19 First, it is not readily apparent whether the mean amount saved (Panel A) differs between the self-help peer groups and the high interest accounts. Especially in the beginning, savings are very similar. In the fourth quarter of the first year, balances are higher in the self-help peer groups compared to high interest rate. A second interesting observation is that mean savings in the high interest rate are not substantially higher than in the basic account indicating that people did not react much to the substantial interest rate increase. Thirdly, Panels B-D show that while the difference between self-help peer groups and high interest accounts on mean savings is not substantial, looking at the whole distribution reveals much starker results. Panel B shows that the 75th percentile of balances is substantially higher for self-help groups than for the increased interest rate. Even at the 95th percentile of savings, people seem not to react to an interest rate increase at all (Panel C). Only at the very top of the distribution (Panel D for the 99 percentile) does the interest rate lead to higher savings. In sum, Figure 2 indicates that self-help peer groups significantly shift the distribution of savings and a substantial increase in the interest occurs only at the very right tail of the savings distribution. Table 3 supports those findings in regressions. The table provides the effect of the Interest Rate treatment compared to the control group. It also shows the p-value of a F -test that compares the effect of Self Help and Interest Rate. The table shows that the high interest rate is not able to significantly increase the average number of deposits. In most specifications, a substantial interest rate increase is also not able to significantly increase the mean amount deposited or the mean savings balance compared to the basic account. While the high interest rate account increases the savings balance by 50%, the increase is not statistically significant. And for the winsorized amounts, which reduce the importance of extreme values, the interest rate increase never has a substantial effect. For all outcomes, the self-help peer groups are much more successful in increasing savings. Comparing the self-help peer groups 19

20 with the interest rate increase shows that the difference is statistically significant for number of deposits and for the winsorized savings amounts, indicating that except for a small number of individuals, the self-help peer groups are more successful. An increase of almost five percentage points in the real interest rate does not increase savings as much as a peer-based commitment device. If we take the results from Column (5) and linearly extrapolate the point estimation of the interest rate increase, the results indicate that the self-help peer groups have an effect equivalent to an increase in the interest rate to 7.8%. The finding that an increase in the interest rate of almost 5 percentage points (from 0.3% to 5%) has very little impact on savings and no impact on most of the distribution is striking in itself. Even individuals who have substantial savings in other savings accounts do not reallocate their money into the high interest account (which is designed such that there is no similarly attractive interest rate commonly available in the Chilean market). Based on the pre-experiment survey, about 32% of individuals have savings in a pre-existing account. If we split those with pre-existing savings further into two groups at the median savings balance, we can investigate whether high pre-treatment savers transfer their money to the high interest bearing account. Savings in the high interest accounts is not higher for high pre-treatment savers than for people with no or low savings in other accounts and the savings are orders of magnitude lower in the account of the study than in the high savers preexisting accounts. This indicates that the high interest rate do not lead to savings reallocation. Indeed, when asked in a survey one year after they received the savings account whether they made any transfers from their pre-existing account into the account they received in the context of this study, only 1% indicate that they did so. There are many potential reasons for why individuals do not react to an increase in interest rate and why people who have savings in other accounts do not make transfers to the high-interest bearing account. Savings in other accounts might not be liquid; 20

21 savings in different accounts may be mentally accounted for different purposes, etc. A series of questions in our follow-up survey allow for more informed speculation about potential reasons. A very prominent candidate for why participants do not react to the high interest rate and do not reallocate money from a lower interest bearing account seems to be a poor understanding of interest rates. Only 2% say that they know what the interest rate is in the alternative savings account. 6 Asked to indicate which of the two accounts had a higher interest rate, 63% of those in the high interest rate treatment nevertheless think that their alternative savings account has a higher interest rate. This is extremely unlikely, as with a 5% higher interest rate than the basic savings account, the high-interest rate account in the savings accounts provided through this study was chosen in such a way as to represent a much higher interest rate than other alternatives commonly available in the Chilean market. The fact that participants seem to have a very weak grasp of the concept of interest rates and of the returns of their respective savings accounts is especially interesting given that in the context of this experiment, the higher interest rate was made exceptionally salient. Before opening an account, participants received a onehour training, explaining the effect of interest rates and compounded interest rates in a very intuitive manner, and showing them that the savings account they were being offered represented the highest alternative in the market. Our finding that even in this situation, participants seemed very unaware of the relative returns of their savings accounts, is an indication that the general population of a similar background to those analyzed in this study cannot be expected to have much command of the notion of interest rates or relative returns. Models or policies based on the assumption that low-income individuals will respond to changes in interest rates should therefore be 6 The questions in this section of the paper were asked of those participants who had opened a savings account in the context of this study and indicated in the follow-up survey one year later that they also had an additional savings account. For participants who indicated having more than one savings account, the questions of this section were asked about the savings account in which they indicated having the largest amount of savings. 21

22 treated with caution. This finding illustrates another advantage of self-help peer groups compared to more traditional approaches: they require little financial literacy. In sum, our first experiment provides causal evidence that self-help peer groups work. Self-help peer groups increase the savings balanced by about 100%. Self-help peer groups work much better in increasing savings than a substantial increase in the interest rate. The follow-up surveys suggest that many individuals have a poor understanding of interest rate. Self-help peer groups are therefore ideal not only in settings in which traditional means of increasing savings, such as defaults in company sponsored savings plans, are not available. They also require much less financial sophistication. 4 The Importance of Peers and Peer Pressure The previous section established that self-help peer group meetings are effective at increasing savings. However, such meetings are also very costly in the sense that people have to physically meet their peers on a regular basis. This section investigates whether such physical peer meetings and peer pressure are actually the crucial elements of what makes self-help peer groups effective in raising savings. This is also the first attempt to unbundle the effect of self-help peer groups. The text-message experiment provides evidence on these questions. We first analyze whether regular follow-up in synthetic peer group meetings through peerrelated text messages can achieve similar results to the regular follow up in physical peer group meetings. We then investigate how important actual peer pressure is for the effect, by comparing two different types of text message follow-up services. In particular, we study whether regular follow-ups combined with being observed by others affects savings more than regular follow-ups combined with information about savings performance of others. 22

23 4.1 The Effect of Peer Text Messages on Savings Figure 3 shows the impact of being assigned to receive a peer-related text message compared to the control group. The horizontal axis represents calendar months in the year 2009, and the area between the horizontal lines marks the period during which the text message intervention was implemented (called Intervention Period going forward). Panel A in Figure 3 shows the average number of deposits per month, and Panel B shows the average amount deposited. [Figure 3] Figure 3 reveals three important results: First, before the experiment begins in August 2009 (month 8), there is no significant difference between treatment and control group in both panels. Deposits in June and July trend slightly downward in the cold winter months in Chile, but this trend is no different between treatment and control. Second, in the months in which the treatment group received peer savings text messages, savings outcomes are substantially different between the treatment and the control groups. The number of deposits per month is substantially higher in the treatment group compared to the control group (Panel A). The treatment almost triples the number of weekly deposits. The amounts deposited (Panel B) are much more noisy to measure, but even there, we see a substantial difference between individuals in the treatment vs. the control group in August and September, while the difference in October is smaller. 7 Third, after the text messages stop, the savings rate of the treated groups decreases, and the savings behavior looks very similar again across groups. This indi- 7 Since the amounts of savings are very noisy due to a small number of large deposits, the amounts depicted in the graph are winsorized at the 5% level. In the regressions that follow, we indicate results with winsorized and unwinsorized amounts. 23

24 cates that while the weekly messages had a strong effect of encouraging individuals to make deposits, there are no long-run effects of the treatment on deposit patterns. In order to estimate the significance of the treatment effect on savings, we estimate regressions of the following general form: S i = α + β 1 T reatment i + P rior Savings i + ɛ i (2) where S i is the savings outcome for individual i during the intervention period. T reatment is a dummy variable equal to 1 for individuals in the treatment groups and 0 otherwise. In addition, we include individual average savings level prior to the intervention period. While prior savings is not significantly different between treatments due to the randomization, controlling for it reduces much of the noise. Prior savings is predictive of savings in the intervention period for multiple reasons. On the one hand, mechanically, people who have prior savings can withdraw more in the intervention period leading to possible negative net savings in the intervention period while people with no prior savings cannot have net withdrawals. On the other hand, those with higher price savings may be more positively predisposed to use the savings account and may be richer, and so can respond more slowly to the text messages. We use three measures for S i : (1) average number of monthly deposits made, (2) average monthly amount deposited in Chilean Pesos, and (3) total savings (depositswithdrawals) in the intervention period. The amounts deposited or saved are shown as means and winsorized at top 1% or top 5%. Table 4 presents the results of OLS regressions for all three outcome measures during the intervention period. Panel A shows the ITT effect while Panel B shows the TOT effect. The results indicate that peer related text messages have a substan- 24

25 tial effect on savings. In the ITT specification, the average number of deposits is more than two times higher in the text message treatments than in the control group. Not only do people deposit more often, they also deposit higher amounts. The average monthly deposited amount is about 2,000 Pesos higher in the treatment group compared to the control. As a result, participants in the treatment group increase their savings balance in the intervention period by about 7,800 Pesos. This increase constitutes about 7% of the average monthly income. Obviously, TOT effect is even higher. Given that take-up is about 42%, the TOT effects are slightly more than double in size. For balances, that implies that treated individuals saved, on average, around 19,000 Pesos or 38 USD in three months compared to 0 for the control group. 8 [Table 4 about here.] To approximate how much of the self-help peer groups effects (with physical meetings) can be achieved with only peer-related text messages, we compare the effect of Experiment 1 with the effect of Experiment 2. Since it is not randomly assigned who participates in the text message experiment, however, the comparison will only give a rough approximation. First, we can simply compare the ITT effects in Table 3 and in Table 4, where we see that the text message treatment has about the same if not a stronger effect than self-help peer groups. However, this comparison is problematic as participants in the text message experiment are a select subsample of the sample of the peer group experiment: they all took up a bank account as part of the study, they have a cell phone, and they expressed interest in a text message follow-up service to help them save more. In addition, the effect in Experiment 1 is for 12 months after the experiment started the intervention period of the text message experiment is only 3 months. 8 The net new savings of zero in the control group indicates that participants in this group on average withdrew the same amount as they deposited, with net new savings negative for some participants during this period. 25

Under-Savers Anonymous Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device

Under-Savers Anonymous Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device Under-Savers Anonymous Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device Felipe Kast Stephan Meier Dina Pomeranz November 2011 PRELIMINARY. DO NOT CIRCULATE. Abstract While

More information

Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device

Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device Felipe Kast Stephan Meier Dina Pomeranz Working Paper 12-060 June 30, 2014 Copyright 2012, 2014 by

More information

UNDER-SAVERS ANONYMOUS: EVIDENCE ON SELF-HELP GROUPS AND PEER PRESSURE AS A SAVINGS COMMITMENT DEVICE

UNDER-SAVERS ANONYMOUS: EVIDENCE ON SELF-HELP GROUPS AND PEER PRESSURE AS A SAVINGS COMMITMENT DEVICE UNDER-SAVERS ANONYMOUS: EVIDENCE ON SELF-HELP GROUPS AND PEER PRESSURE AS A SAVINGS COMMITMENT DEVICE Felipe Kast Stephan Meier Dina Pomeranz December 2011 While commitment devices such as defaults and

More information

Saving More to Borrow Less: Experimental Evidence from Access to Formal Savings Accounts in Chile

Saving More to Borrow Less: Experimental Evidence from Access to Formal Savings Accounts in Chile Saving More to Borrow Less: Experimental Evidence from Access to Formal Savings Accounts in Chile Felipe Kast Dina Pomeranz Working Paper 14-001 June 12, 2014 Copyright 2013, 2014 by Felipe Kast and Dina

More information

Using Behavioral Economics to Improve Diversification in 401(k) Plans: Solving the Company Stock Problem

Using Behavioral Economics to Improve Diversification in 401(k) Plans: Solving the Company Stock Problem Using Behavioral Economics to Improve Diversification in 401(k) Plans: Solving the Company Stock Problem Shlomo Benartzi The Anderson School at UCLA Richard H. Thaler The University of Chicago Investors

More information

MetLife Retirement Income. A Survey of Pre-Retiree Knowledge of Financial Retirement Issues

MetLife Retirement Income. A Survey of Pre-Retiree Knowledge of Financial Retirement Issues MetLife Retirement Income IQ Study A Survey of Pre-Retiree Knowledge of Financial Retirement Issues June, 2008 The MetLife Mature Market Institute Established in 1997, the Mature Market Institute (MMI)

More information

So what s a state to do? Some recent research provides guidance.

So what s a state to do? Some recent research provides guidance. Duncombe.005 The Benefits and Costs of School District Consolidation: What recent research reveals about potential cost savings BY WILLIAM D. DUNCOMBE AND JOHN M. YINGER School district consolidation is

More information

Banking with Agents: Evidence from Senegal. Sinja Buri, IFC Robert Cull, World Bank Xavier Giné, World Bank Sven Harten, IFC

Banking with Agents: Evidence from Senegal. Sinja Buri, IFC Robert Cull, World Bank Xavier Giné, World Bank Sven Harten, IFC Banking with Agents: Evidence from Senegal Sinja Buri, IFC Robert Cull, World Bank Xavier Giné, World Bank Sven Harten, IFC 1 Motivation: 77 percent of adults living on less than $2 a day report not having

More information

An Empirical Analysis of Insider Rates vs. Outsider Rates in Bank Lending

An Empirical Analysis of Insider Rates vs. Outsider Rates in Bank Lending An Empirical Analysis of Insider Rates vs. Outsider Rates in Bank Lending Lamont Black* Indiana University Federal Reserve Board of Governors November 2006 ABSTRACT: This paper analyzes empirically the

More information

BUSINESS AND FINANCIAL LITERACY FOR YOUNG ENTREPRENEURS: EVIDENCE FROM BOSNIA-HERZEGOVINA. Miriam Bruhn and Bilal Zia (World Bank, DECFP)

BUSINESS AND FINANCIAL LITERACY FOR YOUNG ENTREPRENEURS: EVIDENCE FROM BOSNIA-HERZEGOVINA. Miriam Bruhn and Bilal Zia (World Bank, DECFP) BUSINESS AND FINANCIAL LITERACY FOR YOUNG ENTREPRENEURS: EVIDENCE FROM BOSNIA-HERZEGOVINA Miriam Bruhn and Bilal Zia (World Bank, DECFP) Introduction What are the determinants of firm growth? Much of the

More information

Sticking to Your Plan: Hyperbolic Discounting and Credit Card Debt Paydown

Sticking to Your Plan: Hyperbolic Discounting and Credit Card Debt Paydown Sticking to Your Plan: Hyperbolic Discounting and Credit Card Debt Paydown Theresa Kuchler Abstract Using detailed data from an online service, I analyze the influence of present bias on debt paydown behavior.

More information

THE RESPONSIBILITY TO SAVE AND CONTRIBUTE TO

THE RESPONSIBILITY TO SAVE AND CONTRIBUTE TO PREPARING FOR RETIREMENT: THE IMPORTANCE OF PLANNING COSTS Annamaria Lusardi, Dartmouth College* THE RESPONSIBILITY TO SAVE AND CONTRIBUTE TO a pension is increasingly left to the individual worker. For

More information

Percentage. Source: [1] (Malawi), [2] (Kenya), [3] (Chile), and [4] (Nepal).

Percentage. Source: [1] (Malawi), [2] (Kenya), [3] (Chile), and [4] (Nepal). Jessica Goldberg University of Maryland, USA Products and policies to promote saving in developing countries Combine behavioral insights with good products to increase formal savings in developing countries

More information

When is capital enough to get female microenterprises growing? Evidence from a randomized experiment in Ghana * Marcel Fafchamps, University of Oxford

When is capital enough to get female microenterprises growing? Evidence from a randomized experiment in Ghana * Marcel Fafchamps, University of Oxford When is capital enough to get female microenterprises growing? Evidence from a randomized experiment in Ghana * Marcel Fafchamps, University of Oxford David McKenzie, World Bank Simon Quinn, University

More information

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES REMEMBERING TO PAY? REMINDERS VS. FINANCIAL INCENTIVES FOR LOAN PAYMENTS. Ximena Cadena Antoinette Schoar

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES REMEMBERING TO PAY? REMINDERS VS. FINANCIAL INCENTIVES FOR LOAN PAYMENTS. Ximena Cadena Antoinette Schoar NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES REMEMBERING TO PAY? REMINDERS VS. FINANCIAL INCENTIVES FOR LOAN PAYMENTS Ximena Cadena Antoinette Schoar Working Paper 17020 http://www.nber.org/papers/w17020 NATIONAL BUREAU

More information

Child Support Demonstration Evaluation Cost-Benefit Analysis, September 1997-December 2004

Child Support Demonstration Evaluation Cost-Benefit Analysis, September 1997-December 2004 Child Support Demonstration Evaluation Cost-Benefit Analysis, September 1997-December 2004 Emma Caspar and Steven Cook Institute for Research on Poverty University of Wisconsin Madison March 2006 Report

More information

Saving and Life Insurance Holdings at Boston University A Unique Case Study

Saving and Life Insurance Holdings at Boston University A Unique Case Study Saving and Life Insurance Holdings at Boston University A Unique Case Study by B. Douglas Bernheim Stanford University and National Bureau of Economic Research bernheim@stanford.edu Solange Berstein Boston

More information

What Does Reputation Buy? Differentiation in a Market for Third-party Auditors

What Does Reputation Buy? Differentiation in a Market for Third-party Auditors What Does Reputation Buy? Differentiation in a Market for Third-party Auditors By ESTHER DUFLO, MICHAEL GREENSTONE, ROHINI PANDE AND NICHOLAS RYAN* * Duflo: MIT Department of Economics, 50 Memorial Dr.,

More information

Remembering to Pay? Reminders vs. Financial Incentives for Loan Payments

Remembering to Pay? Reminders vs. Financial Incentives for Loan Payments Remembering to Pay? Reminders vs. Financial Incentives for Loan Payments Ximena Cadena, ideas42 Antoinette Schoar, MIT, ideas42 and NBER 1 Abstract We report the results from a field experiment with a

More information

Raising the Bar: Pumping Up Retirement Savings

Raising the Bar: Pumping Up Retirement Savings Raising the Bar: Pumping Up Retirement Savings A Research Report by DCIIA 1 Summary Americans are saving in defined contribution plans such as 401(k)s with the understanding that these assets will provide

More information

Volume URL: http://www.nber.org/books/feld87-2. Chapter Title: Individual Retirement Accounts and Saving

Volume URL: http://www.nber.org/books/feld87-2. Chapter Title: Individual Retirement Accounts and Saving This PDF is a selection from an out-of-print volume from the National Bureau of Economic Research Volume Title: Taxes and Capital Formation Volume Author/Editor: Martin Feldstein, ed. Volume Publisher:

More information

BANKING THE POOR VIA SAVINGS ACCOUNTS: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT. Silvia Prina

BANKING THE POOR VIA SAVINGS ACCOUNTS: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT. Silvia Prina BANKING THE POOR VIA SAVINGS ACCOUNTS: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT Silvia Prina January 28, 2013 Abstract The majority of the poor lack access to bank accounts and have to use costly informal savings

More information

The Role of Time Preferences and Exponential-Growth Bias in Retirement Savings

The Role of Time Preferences and Exponential-Growth Bias in Retirement Savings The Role of Time Preferences and Exponential-Growth Bias in Retirement Savings Gopi Shah Goda Stanford University and NBER Colleen Flaherty Manchester University of Minnesota Matthew R. Levy London School

More information

Technical Report - Practical measurements of Security Systems

Technical Report - Practical measurements of Security Systems A Short Study on Security Indicator Interfaces Technical Report UCSC-WASP-15-04 November 2015 D J Capelis mail@capelis.dj Working-group on Applied Security and Privacy Storage Systems Research Center University

More information

Abstract Title Page Not included in page count.

Abstract Title Page Not included in page count. Abstract Title Page Not included in page count. Title: The Impact of The Stock Market Game on Financial Literacy and Mathematics Achievement: Results from a National Randomized Controlled Trial. Author(s):

More information

What Goes Up Must Come Down? Experimental Evidence on Intuitive Forecasting

What Goes Up Must Come Down? Experimental Evidence on Intuitive Forecasting What Goes Up Must Come Down? Experimental Evidence on Intuitive Forecasting BY JOHN BESHEARS, JAMES J. CHOI, ANDREAS FUSTER, DAVID LAIBSON, AND BRIGITTE C. MADRIAN Abstract: Do laboratory subjects correctly

More information

Competitive Bids and Post-Issuance Price Performance in the Municipal Bond Market. Daniel Bergstresser* Randolph Cohen**

Competitive Bids and Post-Issuance Price Performance in the Municipal Bond Market. Daniel Bergstresser* Randolph Cohen** Competitive Bids and Post-Issuance Price Performance in the Municipal Bond Market Daniel Bergstresser* Randolph Cohen** (March 2015. Comments welcome. Please do not cite or distribute without permission)

More information

AN EXPERIMENT IN CALL SCHEDULING. Patricia Cunningham, David Martin, J. Michael Brick Westat, 1650 Research Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850-3195

AN EXPERIMENT IN CALL SCHEDULING. Patricia Cunningham, David Martin, J. Michael Brick Westat, 1650 Research Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850-3195 AN EXPERIMENT IN CALL SCHEDULING Patricia Cunningham, David Martin, J. Michael Brick Westat, 1650 Research Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850-3195 Keywords: Random Digit Dial, Telephone Survey, Calling Protocol

More information

Household Decision Making and Savings Impacts: Further Evidence from a Commitment Savings Product in the Philippines.

Household Decision Making and Savings Impacts: Further Evidence from a Commitment Savings Product in the Philippines. Household Decision Making and Savings Impacts: Further Evidence from a Commitment Savings Product in the Philippines May 28 th, 2006 Nava Ashraf Dean Karlan Wesley Yin Yale University dean.karlan@yale.edu

More information

Alternative Retirement Financial Plans and Their Features

Alternative Retirement Financial Plans and Their Features RETIREMENT ACCOUNTS Gary R. Evans, 2006-2013, November 20, 2013. The various retirement investment accounts discussed in this document all offer the potential for healthy longterm returns with substantial

More information

Inflation. Chapter 8. 8.1 Money Supply and Demand

Inflation. Chapter 8. 8.1 Money Supply and Demand Chapter 8 Inflation This chapter examines the causes and consequences of inflation. Sections 8.1 and 8.2 relate inflation to money supply and demand. Although the presentation differs somewhat from that

More information

January 2015. Report on SBLF Participants Small Business Lending Growth Submitted to Congress pursuant to Section 4106(3) of

January 2015. Report on SBLF Participants Small Business Lending Growth Submitted to Congress pursuant to Section 4106(3) of January 2015 Report on SBLF Participants Small Business Lending Growth Submitted to Congress pursuant to Section 4106(3) of the Small Business Jobs Act of 2010 OVERVIEW Small businesses are a vital part

More information

Insights: American Financial Capability

Insights: American Financial Capability A REPORT FROM THE FINRA INVESTOR EDUCATION FOUNDATION Insights: American Financial Capability APRIL 2012 Author: Gary R. Mottola, Ph.D. What s Inside: Summary 1 Background 1 Gender and Credit Card Behavior

More information

AN INTRODUCTION TO PREMIUM TREND

AN INTRODUCTION TO PREMIUM TREND AN INTRODUCTION TO PREMIUM TREND Burt D. Jones * February, 2002 Acknowledgement I would like to acknowledge the valuable assistance of Catherine Taylor, who was instrumental in the development of this

More information

Online Appendix: Thar SHE blows? Gender, Competition, and Bubbles in Experimental Asset Markets, by Catherine C. Eckel and Sascha C.

Online Appendix: Thar SHE blows? Gender, Competition, and Bubbles in Experimental Asset Markets, by Catherine C. Eckel and Sascha C. Online Appendix: Thar SHE blows? Gender, Competition, and Bubbles in Experimental Asset Markets, by Catherine C. Eckel and Sascha C. Füllbrunn A1. META-ANALYSIS Treatment TABLE A1. BUBBLE MEASURES FOR

More information

4. Conducting performance monitoring and evaluation

4. Conducting performance monitoring and evaluation 60 4. Conducting performance monitoring and evaluation IFAD DECISION TOOLS FOR RURAL FINANCE 4. Conducting performance monitoring and evaluation Action: Effectively conduct ongoing and annual performance

More information

USEFUL LUMP SUMS: MICROENTERPRISE REVENUE MANAGEMENT AND ITS POTENTIAL FOR BANKS

USEFUL LUMP SUMS: MICROENTERPRISE REVENUE MANAGEMENT AND ITS POTENTIAL FOR BANKS FINANCIAL SERVICES ASSESSMENT USEFUL LUMP SUMS: MICROENTERPRISE REVENUE MANAGEMENT AND ITS POTENTIAL FOR BANKS s The median microentrepreneur household had to handle $2.31 of business-related cash to generate

More information

TYING ODYSSEUS TO THE MAST: EVIDENCE FROM A COMMITMENT SAVINGS PRODUCT IN THE PHILIPPINES

TYING ODYSSEUS TO THE MAST: EVIDENCE FROM A COMMITMENT SAVINGS PRODUCT IN THE PHILIPPINES ECONOMIC GROWTH CENTER YALE UNIVERSITY P.O. Box 208629 New Haven, CT 06520-8269 http://www.econ.yale.edu/~egcenter/ CENTER DISCUSSION PAPER NO. 917 TYING ODYSSEUS TO THE MAST: EVIDENCE FROM A COMMITMENT

More information

Do Micro-Entrepreneurship Programs Increase Wage-Work? Evidence from Chile 1 Claudia Martínez A. 2 Esteban Puentes 3 Jaime Ruiz-Tagle 4

Do Micro-Entrepreneurship Programs Increase Wage-Work? Evidence from Chile 1 Claudia Martínez A. 2 Esteban Puentes 3 Jaime Ruiz-Tagle 4 Do Micro-Entrepreneurship Programs Increase Wage-Work? Evidence from Chile 1 Claudia Martínez A. 2 Esteban Puentes 3 Jaime Ruiz-Tagle 4 This Version: June 2015 Abstract Using a randomized controlled trial

More information

Life Cycle Asset Allocation A Suitable Approach for Defined Contribution Pension Plans

Life Cycle Asset Allocation A Suitable Approach for Defined Contribution Pension Plans Life Cycle Asset Allocation A Suitable Approach for Defined Contribution Pension Plans Challenges for defined contribution plans While Eastern Europe is a prominent example of the importance of defined

More information

Chapter 1 Introduction

Chapter 1 Introduction Chapter 1 Introduction 1.1 Aim of the study Development economists and policy makers generally identify access to credit as one of the main determinants of economic activity and alleviation of poverty

More information

Liquidity in U.S. Treasury spot and futures markets

Liquidity in U.S. Treasury spot and futures markets Liquidity in U.S. Treasury spot and futures markets Michael Fleming and Asani Sarkar* Federal Reserve Bank of New York 33 Liberty Street New York, NY 10045 (212) 720-6372 (Fleming) (212) 720-8943 (Sarkar)

More information

Introduction to Rapid-Fire Operational Testing for Social Programs

Introduction to Rapid-Fire Operational Testing for Social Programs GOLDILOCKS DEEP DIVE Introduction to Rapid-Fire Operational Testing for Social Programs Copyright 2016 Innovations for Poverty Action. Introduction to Rapid-Fire Operational Testing for Social Programs

More information

Jessica S. Banthin and Thomas M. Selden. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Working Paper No. 06005. July 2006

Jessica S. Banthin and Thomas M. Selden. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Working Paper No. 06005. July 2006 Income Measurement in the Medical Expenditure Panel Survey Jessica S. Banthin and Thomas M. Selden Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Working Paper No. 06005 July 2006 Suggested citation: Banthin

More information

Belief Formation in the Returns to Schooling

Belief Formation in the Returns to Schooling Working paper Belief Formation in the Returns to ing Jim Berry Lucas Coffman March 212 Belief Formation in the Returns to ing March 31, 212 Jim Berry Cornell University Lucas Coffman Ohio State U. & Yale

More information

By Annamaria Lusardi and Olivia S. Mitchell*

By Annamaria Lusardi and Olivia S. Mitchell* American Economic Review: Papers & Proceedings 2008, 98:2, 413 417 http://www.aeaweb.org/articles.php?doi 10.1257/aer.98.2.413 By Annamaria Lusardi and Olivia S. Mitchell* Many baby boomers are approaching

More information

Berkeley CS191x: Quantum Mechanics and Quantum Computation Optional Class Project

Berkeley CS191x: Quantum Mechanics and Quantum Computation Optional Class Project Berkeley CS191x: Quantum Mechanics and Quantum Computation Optional Class Project This document describes the optional class project for the Fall 2013 offering of CS191x. The project will not be graded.

More information

Saving For Retirement? Start by Paying Off Your Credit Cards

Saving For Retirement? Start by Paying Off Your Credit Cards Saving For Retirement? Start by Paying Off Your Credit Cards David Blanchett, CFA, CFP Head of Retirement Research Morningstar s Investment Management division August 16, 2012 Introduction Credit cards

More information

Introduction1. Sample Description. Drivers of Costs and the Empirical Approach or Explanatory Variables:

Introduction1. Sample Description. Drivers of Costs and the Empirical Approach or Explanatory Variables: Efficiency Drivers of Microfinance Institutions (MFIs): The Case of Operating Costs 1 Adrian Gonzalez, Researcher, MIX (agonzalez@themix.org) The findings, interpretations, and conclusions expressed in

More information

43% of all American households are liquid asset poor

43% of all American households are liquid asset poor Project Evaluation: Commitment Savings Fighting Poverty with Innovation, Evidence and Action Saving money can be difficult, and growing evidence suggests that self-control issues may contribute to undersaving.

More information

Credit Card Market Study Interim Report: Annex 4 Switching Analysis

Credit Card Market Study Interim Report: Annex 4 Switching Analysis MS14/6.2: Annex 4 Market Study Interim Report: Annex 4 November 2015 This annex describes data analysis we carried out to improve our understanding of switching and shopping around behaviour in the UK

More information

Tracking The Long-Term Impact of Incentive Programs Using Readily Available Employee Data

Tracking The Long-Term Impact of Incentive Programs Using Readily Available Employee Data Measurement series 4.7 Tracking The Long-Term Impact of Incentive Programs Using Readily Available Employee Data FOCUS A variety of existing data is probably already available to support the measurement

More information

Chicago Booth BUSINESS STATISTICS 41000 Final Exam Fall 2011

Chicago Booth BUSINESS STATISTICS 41000 Final Exam Fall 2011 Chicago Booth BUSINESS STATISTICS 41000 Final Exam Fall 2011 Name: Section: I pledge my honor that I have not violated the Honor Code Signature: This exam has 34 pages. You have 3 hours to complete this

More information

Determinants of Stock Market Performance in Pakistan

Determinants of Stock Market Performance in Pakistan Determinants of Stock Market Performance in Pakistan Mehwish Zafar Sr. Lecturer Bahria University, Karachi campus Abstract Stock market performance, economic and political condition of a country is interrelated

More information

PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME

PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME CHAPTER- V PERCEPTION OF SENIOR CITIZEN RESPONDENTS AS TO REVERSE MORTGAGE SCHEME 5.1 Introduction The present study intended to investigate the senior citizen s retirement planning and their perception

More information

One method of measuring the prices of deposit and loan services is to view the differentials

One method of measuring the prices of deposit and loan services is to view the differentials On Indirect Measurement Methods of Deposit and Loan Service Prices - Using Published Interest Rate Data to Measure Deposit and Loan Services Prices, and Problems with this Method - Toru Ohmori Economic

More information

The Cost of Convenience? Transaction Costs, Bargaining Power, and Savings Account Use in Kenya

The Cost of Convenience? Transaction Costs, Bargaining Power, and Savings Account Use in Kenya The Cost of Convenience? Transaction Costs, Bargaining Power, and Savings Account Use in Kenya Simone Schaner Dartmouth 6 September, 2013 Savings in the Developing World Pervasive lack of access to formal

More information

Mortgages and Loans. Huonville: 8/16 Main St, Huonville 7109 DX 70754, Huonville PO Box 239, Huonville 7109 Ph: 03 6264 2967

Mortgages and Loans. Huonville: 8/16 Main St, Huonville 7109 DX 70754, Huonville PO Box 239, Huonville 7109 Ph: 03 6264 2967 Mortgages and Loans Huonville: 8/16 Main St, Huonville 7109 DX 70754, Huonville PO Box 239, Huonville 7109 Ph: 03 6264 2967 Hobart: Level 1, 18 Elizabeth St, Hobart 7000 DX 231, Hobart GPO Box 16, Hobart

More information

How To Understand How A Bellcurve Works

How To Understand How A Bellcurve Works Punishing by Rewards: When the Bellcurve Stops Working For You Chintan Vaishnav chintanv@mit.edu Engineering Systems Division Massachusetts Institute of Technology Ali Khakifirooz khaki@mit.edu Department

More information

The Impact of Consulting Services on Small and Medium Enterprises: Evidence from a Randomized Trial in Mexico 1

The Impact of Consulting Services on Small and Medium Enterprises: Evidence from a Randomized Trial in Mexico 1 The Impact of Consulting Services on Small and Medium Enterprises: Evidence from a Randomized Trial in Mexico 1 Miriam Bruhn mbruhn@worldbank.org The World Bank Dean Karlan dean.karlan@yale.edu Yale University;

More information

MEASURING INCOME DYNAMICS: The Experience of Canada s Survey of Labour and Income Dynamics

MEASURING INCOME DYNAMICS: The Experience of Canada s Survey of Labour and Income Dynamics CANADA CANADA 2 MEASURING INCOME DYNAMICS: The Experience of Canada s Survey of Labour and Income Dynamics by Maryanne Webber Statistics Canada Canada for presentation at Seminar on Poverty Statistics

More information

A Primer on Forecasting Business Performance

A Primer on Forecasting Business Performance A Primer on Forecasting Business Performance There are two common approaches to forecasting: qualitative and quantitative. Qualitative forecasting methods are important when historical data is not available.

More information

The Risk of Losing Health Insurance Over a Decade: New Findings from Longitudinal Data. Executive Summary

The Risk of Losing Health Insurance Over a Decade: New Findings from Longitudinal Data. Executive Summary The Risk of Losing Health Insurance Over a Decade: New Findings from Longitudinal Data Executive Summary It is often assumed that policies to make health insurance more affordable to the uninsured would

More information

Internet Appendix for. Incentivizing Calculated Risk-Taking: Evidence from an Experiment with Commercial Bank Loan Officers

Internet Appendix for. Incentivizing Calculated Risk-Taking: Evidence from an Experiment with Commercial Bank Loan Officers Internet Appendix for Incentivizing Calculated Risk-Taking: Evidence from an Experiment with Commercial Bank Loan Officers SHAWN COLE, MARTIN KANZ, and LEORA KLAPPER This Appendix presents additional materials

More information

Construction of variables from CE

Construction of variables from CE Online Appendix for Has Consumption Inequality Mirrored Income Inequality? by Mark Aguiar and Mark Bils In this appendix we describe construction of the variables in our data set and the impact of sample

More information

The Real Impact of Improved Access to Finance: Evidence from Mexico

The Real Impact of Improved Access to Finance: Evidence from Mexico The Real Impact of Improved Access to Finance: Evidence from Mexico MIRIAM BRUHN AND INESSA LOVE * Article forthcoming in The Journal of Finance ABSTRACT This paper provides new evidence on the impact

More information

RETIREMENT ACCOUNTS (c) Gary R. Evans, 2006-2011, September 24, 2011. Alternative Retirement Financial Plans and Their Features

RETIREMENT ACCOUNTS (c) Gary R. Evans, 2006-2011, September 24, 2011. Alternative Retirement Financial Plans and Their Features RETIREMENT ACCOUNTS (c) Gary R. Evans, 2006-2011, September 24, 2011. The various retirement investment accounts discussed in this document all offer the potential for healthy longterm returns with substantial

More information

To Have and to Hold: An Analysis of Young Adult Debt

To Have and to Hold: An Analysis of Young Adult Debt September 2006, Issue 33 To Have and to Hold: An Analysis of Young Adult Debt Ngina S. Chiteji Today's young adults often have been characterized as a generation of borrowers. But are they any different

More information

Tax time provides a unique opportunity for people

Tax time provides a unique opportunity for people Special Focus: Unemployment Financial Education Tax Time as an Asset Building Opportunity Assessing the Potential By J. Michael Collins, University of Wisconsin-Madison Tax time provides a unique opportunity

More information

Chapter 21: The Discounted Utility Model

Chapter 21: The Discounted Utility Model Chapter 21: The Discounted Utility Model 21.1: Introduction This is an important chapter in that it introduces, and explores the implications of, an empirically relevant utility function representing intertemporal

More information

Learning to Save, Saving to Learn

Learning to Save, Saving to Learn Learning to Save, Saving to Learn Individual Development Accounts Project Final Report: Highlights Norm Leckie Taylor Shek-Wai Hui Doug Tattrie Jennifer Robson Jean-Pierre Voyer A project sponsored by

More information

The Basics of Interest Theory

The Basics of Interest Theory Contents Preface 3 The Basics of Interest Theory 9 1 The Meaning of Interest................................... 10 2 Accumulation and Amount Functions............................ 14 3 Effective Interest

More information

MACROECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF VARIOUS PROPOSALS TO PROVIDE $500 BILLION IN TAX RELIEF

MACROECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF VARIOUS PROPOSALS TO PROVIDE $500 BILLION IN TAX RELIEF MACROECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF VARIOUS PROPOSALS TO PROVIDE $500 BILLION IN TAX RELIEF Prepared by the Staff of the JOINT COMMITTEE ON TAXATION March 1, 2005 JCX-4-05 CONTENTS INTRODUCTION... 1 EXECUTIVE SUMMARY...

More information

Discussion of Capital Injection, Monetary Policy, and Financial Accelerators

Discussion of Capital Injection, Monetary Policy, and Financial Accelerators Discussion of Capital Injection, Monetary Policy, and Financial Accelerators Karl Walentin Sveriges Riksbank 1. Background This paper is part of the large literature that takes as its starting point the

More information

HELP Interest Rate Options: Equity and Costs

HELP Interest Rate Options: Equity and Costs HELP Interest Rate Options: Equity and Costs Bruce Chapman and Timothy Higgins July 2014 Abstract This document presents analysis and discussion of the implications of bond indexation on HELP debt. This

More information

When Saving is Gambling

When Saving is Gambling When Saving is Gambling J. Anthony Cookson University of Colorado at Boulder FTC Microeconomics Presentation Gambling and Financial Decisions... the difference between having fun and being smart Some investors

More information

The Effect of Peer-Based Financial Education on Financial Decision Making: Experimental Evidence for Foreign Domestic Workers in Singapore

The Effect of Peer-Based Financial Education on Financial Decision Making: Experimental Evidence for Foreign Domestic Workers in Singapore The Effect of Peer-Based Financial Education on Financial Decision Making: Experimental Evidence for Foreign Domestic Workers in Singapore April 2013 Motivation Literature FDWs in Singapore The Intervention

More information

United States. PROJECT EVALUATION: Borrow Less Tomorrow. American households tend to borrow too much and save too little POLICY ISSUE

United States. PROJECT EVALUATION: Borrow Less Tomorrow. American households tend to borrow too much and save too little POLICY ISSUE PROJECT EVALUATION: Borrow Less Tomorrow Fighting Poverty with Innovation, Evidence and Action For many households, debt decumulation is the most efficient way to increase net worth. Borrow Less Tomorrow

More information

BETTER LENDING AND BETTER CLIENTS: CREDIT BUREAU IMPACT ON MICROFINANCE

BETTER LENDING AND BETTER CLIENTS: CREDIT BUREAU IMPACT ON MICROFINANCE BETTER LENDING AND BETTER CLIENTS: CREDIT BUREAU IMPACT ON MICROFINANCE by Alain de Janvry alain@are.berkeley.edu, Jill Luoto luoto@are.berkeley.edu, Craig McIntosh ctmcintosh@ucsd.edu, Greg Rafert rafert@are.berkeley.edu,

More information

Recognizing Informed Option Trading

Recognizing Informed Option Trading Recognizing Informed Option Trading Alex Bain, Prabal Tiwaree, Kari Okamoto 1 Abstract While equity (stock) markets are generally efficient in discounting public information into stock prices, we believe

More information

Demand for Flexibility in Microfinance Loans: Evidence from India

Demand for Flexibility in Microfinance Loans: Evidence from India Demand for Flexibility in Microfinance Loans: Evidence from India Jihae John Hwang Northwestern University June 2011 Abstract Microfinance loans have expanded greatly in the recent years as a way of helping

More information

Savers receive payment in the form of interest earned on deposits in

Savers receive payment in the form of interest earned on deposits in T OOL 2 Calculating Interest on Savings Jesus R. Chavez Savers receive payment in the form of interest earned on deposits in return for allowing the savings institution use of their funds. While many microfinance

More information

Employers costs for total benefits grew

Employers costs for total benefits grew Costs Benefit Costs Comparing benefit costs for full- and part-time workers Health insurance appears to be the only benefit representing a true quasi-fixed cost to employers, meaning that the cost per

More information

The public expenditure and distributional implications of reforming student loans and grants

The public expenditure and distributional implications of reforming student loans and grants The public expenditure and distributional implications of reforming student loans and grants A project for Universities UK Contents Project details Simulating graduates lifetime earnings Assumptions The

More information

Repayment Resource Guide. Planning for Student Success

Repayment Resource Guide. Planning for Student Success Repayment Resource Guide Planning for Student Success 2013 Table of Contents Table of Contents... 1 Introduction... 3 Purpose of Document... 3 Role of Post Secondary Institutions... 3 Consequences of Student

More information

Moral Hazard. Itay Goldstein. Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania

Moral Hazard. Itay Goldstein. Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania Moral Hazard Itay Goldstein Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania 1 Principal-Agent Problem Basic problem in corporate finance: separation of ownership and control: o The owners of the firm are typically

More information

Procrastination, Present-Biased Preferences, and Financial Behaviors

Procrastination, Present-Biased Preferences, and Financial Behaviors Procrastination, Present-Biased Preferences, and Financial Behaviors Jeffrey Brown University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign and Alessandro Previtero University of Western Ontario Prepared for the 16

More information

The Stock Market s Reaction to Accounting Information: The Case of the Latin American Integrated Market. Abstract

The Stock Market s Reaction to Accounting Information: The Case of the Latin American Integrated Market. Abstract The Stock Market s Reaction to Accounting Information: The Case of the Latin American Integrated Market Abstract The purpose of this paper is to explore the stock market s reaction to quarterly financial

More information

Assessing the Effect of Self-Control on Retirement Preparedness of U.S. Households

Assessing the Effect of Self-Control on Retirement Preparedness of U.S. Households Assessing the Effect of Self-Control on Retirement Preparedness of U.S. Households Kyoung Tae Kim, The Ohio State University 1 Jae Min Lee, The Ohio State University 2 Eunice O. Hong, The Ohio State University

More information

Stock Market Game Test

Stock Market Game Test Stock Market Game Test A test of basic economic concepts and institutions related to saving, investing, risk, the stock market, and productivity 1. A personal investment such as purchasing stocks or corporate

More information

How To Calculate A Balance On A Savings Account

How To Calculate A Balance On A Savings Account 319 CHAPTER 4 Personal Finance The following is an article from a Marlboro, Massachusetts newspaper. NEWSPAPER ARTICLE 4.1: LET S TEACH FINANCIAL LITERACY STEPHEN LEDUC WED JAN 16, 2008 Boston - Last week

More information

Friendship at Work: Can Peer Effects Catalyze Female Entrepreneurship?

Friendship at Work: Can Peer Effects Catalyze Female Entrepreneurship? Friendship at Work: Can Peer Effects Catalyze Female Entrepreneurship? By Erica Field, Seema Jayachandran, Rohini Pande, and Natalia Rigol Does lack of peers contribute to the observed gender gap in entrepreneurial

More information

Personal Accounts and Social Security Reform

Personal Accounts and Social Security Reform Preliminary Draft Retirement, Saving, Benefit Claiming and Solvency under a Partial System of Voluntary Personal Accounts Alan L. Gustman Dartmouth College and NBER and Thomas Steinmeier Texas Tech University

More information

ORGANISATIONAL DIAGNOSIS QUESTIONNAIRE (ODQ)

ORGANISATIONAL DIAGNOSIS QUESTIONNAIRE (ODQ) ORGANISATIONAL DIAGNOSIS QUESTIONNAIRE (ODQ) Robert C. Preziosi Both internal and external organization development (OD) consultants at some point in the consulting process must address the question of

More information

In this chapter, you will learn improvement curve concepts and their application to cost and price analysis.

In this chapter, you will learn improvement curve concepts and their application to cost and price analysis. 7.0 - Chapter Introduction In this chapter, you will learn improvement curve concepts and their application to cost and price analysis. Basic Improvement Curve Concept. You may have learned about improvement

More information

Stable matching: Theory, evidence, and practical design

Stable matching: Theory, evidence, and practical design THE PRIZE IN ECONOMIC SCIENCES 2012 INFORMATION FOR THE PUBLIC Stable matching: Theory, evidence, and practical design This year s Prize to Lloyd Shapley and Alvin Roth extends from abstract theory developed

More information

Participant Behavior Insight

Participant Behavior Insight T. Rowe Price Participant Behavior Insight How Participants Are Using Target-Date Funds Brought to you by Jodi DiCenzo, CFA, Behavioral Research Associates, LLC. 1 Retirement Research Executive Summary

More information

SAVING INCENTIVES FOR LOW- AND MIDDLE-INCOME FAMILIES: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT WITH H&R BLOCK *

SAVING INCENTIVES FOR LOW- AND MIDDLE-INCOME FAMILIES: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT WITH H&R BLOCK * SAVING INCENTIVES FOR LOW- AND MIDDLE-INCOME FAMILIES: EVIDENCE FROM A FIELD EXPERIMENT WITH H&R BLOCK * By Esther Duflo William Gale Jeffrey Liebman Peter Orszag Emmanuel Saez Revised, February 2006 Abstract

More information

China s Middle Market for Life Insurance

China s Middle Market for Life Insurance China s Middle Market for Life Insurance May 2014 Sponsored by: SOA International Section SOA Marketing & Distribution Section SOA Research Expanding Boundaries Pool The opinions expressed and conclusions

More information